Much of our thinking occurs in the form of propositions, statements that express ideas. All propositions consist of concepts combined in a particular way. For example, “college students are intelligent people” is a proposition in which the two concepts “college students” and “intelligent people” are linked by the verb are.

Thinking

image source: dolcesoul.com

Concepts are basic units of semantic memory—mental cate­gories into which we place objects, activities, abstractions (such as “liberal” and “conser­vative”), and events that have essential features in common. Every psychological term you are learning in this course is a concept. Concepts can be acquired through explicit instruction or through our own observations of similarities and diffe­rences among various objects and events.

ADVERTISEMENTS:

Many concepts are difficult to define explicitly. For example, you are quite familiar with the concept “vegetable,” yet you might have difficulty coming up with an explicit definition of what a vegetable is. However, you can quickly think of a good example of a vegetable, such as broccoli or carrots.

According to Eleanor Rosch, many concepts are defined by prototypes, the most typical and familiar members of a category, or class. Rosch suggests that we often decide which category something belongs to by its degree of resemblance to the prototype. Consider the follo­wing questions:

Is an eagle a bird?

Is a penguin a bird?

ADVERTISEMENTS:

Is a bat a bird?

According to the prototype view, you should have come to a quicker decision on the first ques­tion than on the last two. Why? Because an eagle fits most people’s “bird” prototype better than does a penguin (which is a bird, though it lacks some essential prototypic features, such as the ability to fly) or a bat (which is not a bird, even though it flies). Experiments measuring how quickly participants responded yes or no to the preceding questions have found that it does in­deed take most people longer to decide whether penguins or bats are birds.

The use of prototypes is perhaps the most ele­mentary method of forming concepts. It requires that we note only similarities among objects. Thus, children’s early concepts are based on prototypes of the objects and people they en­counter person-ally.

They then decide if new objects are similar enough to the prototype to be a “Mommy,” a “cookie,” a “doggie,” and so on. Because proto-types may differ as a result of personal experi-ence, there is considerable room for arbitrariness and individual differences in prototypic concepts. Thus one person’s “terrorist” can be another per-son’s “freedom fighter.”

ADVERTISEMENTS:

How we state propositions can influence, how we try to solve a problem, reason through to a de­cision, or make a judgment. For example, in one study, college students who were told that a cancer treatment had a 50 per cent success rate judged the treatment to be significantly more effective and expressed a greater willingness to have it administered to a family member than did participants who were told that it had a 50 per cent failure rate.

Representing outcomes in terms of positives or negatives has this effect because people tend to assign greater costs to negative outcomes (such as losing $100) than they assign value to an equivalent positive outcome (finding $100). The proposition that “there is a 50 per cent chance of failure” evokes thoughts about the patient’s dying and causes the 50-50 treatment to appear riskier. Thus, differences in how we verbally represent choices and goals can make a difference in our perceptions and decisions.